Publisher Correction: Kindness and happiness at work
December 2025
|
Journal article
|
Discover Psychology
Mapping moral judgments of food waste: Moving beyond truisms
July 2025
|
Journal article
|
Personality and Individual Differences
5202 Biological Psychology, 5205 Social and Personality Psychology, 52 Psychology, 2 Zero Hunger
The Extended Morality as Cooperation Dictionary (eMACD): A Crowd-Sourced Approach via the Moral Narrative Analyzer Platform
January 2025
|
Journal article
|
Communication Methods and Measures
4701 Communication and Media Studies, 47 Language, Communication and Culture
Kindness and happiness at work
November 2024
|
Journal article
|
Discover Psychology
What is the relationship between kindness and happiness at work? Previous research has shown that kindness is a cause and a correlate of happiness in general, and that kinder companies have happier employees. Here we build on this research by using a new measure—the The Kindness Questionnaire (KQ)—to look at the relationship between kindness to and from specific individuals at work, and workplace happiness. In Study 1 (n = 1151) we find that kindness to and from bosses, colleagues and subordinates, and a more general measure of kindness at work, predicts happiness at work. In Study 2 (n = 781) we find that kindness to bosses, colleagues and subordinates, and kindness from bosses, colleagues but not subordinates, as well as more general measures of kindness at work, predict happiness at work. However, in both studies, the simpler general measures of kindness at work are better predictors of happiness at work than the more complicated specific measures. We also find that fair pay, meaningful work, and a competent boss predict happiness at work. The implications, limitations and future directions of this research are discussed.
Kindness, Organizational behavior, Happiness
The costs and benefits of kindness for kids.
October 2024
|
Journal article
|
Journal of experimental child psychology
What do children think makes an act kind? Which kind acts are children likely to perform? Previous research with adults suggests that the kindness of acts depends largely on the benefit provided and to a lesser extent on the cost incurred, and that adults are more likely to perform low-cost, high-benefit kind acts. In the current study, children (9-12 years, n = 945) and teens (13-17 years, n = 939) rated the benefit, cost, kindness, and likelihood of performing 173 acts of kindness, and adults (18+ years, n = 891) rated how beneficial, costly, kind, and likely the acts would be for young people to perform. Among children and teens, benefit but not cost predicted the kindness of acts, and benefit positively predicted, but cost negatively predicted, performance (for "kindness quotients" of 61% and 65%, respectively). Among adults, benefit and cost predicted the kindness of acts, and cost, but not benefit, negatively predicted performance (for a kindness quotient of 59%). The results for children and teens are similar to those from previous research with adults; however, adults are more sensitive to cost when rating kindness, are less sensitive to benefit when rating performance by young people, and are less likely to think young people will perform acts of kindness overall. In practical terms, the results suggest that recommending cost-effective acts may be the best way to encourage children to be kinder.
Humans, Empathy, Age Factors, Adolescent, Adult, Child, Cost-Benefit Analysis, Female, Male, Young Adult
Moral universals: A machine-reading analysis of 256 societies.
March 2024
|
Journal article
|
Heliyon
What is the cross-cultural prevalence of the seven moral values posited by the theory of "morality-as-cooperation"? Previous research, using laborious hand-coding of ethnographic accounts of ethics from 60 societies, found examples of most of the seven morals in most societies, and observed these morals with equal frequency across cultural regions. Here we replicate and extend this analysis by developing a new Morality-as-Cooperation Dictionary (MAC-D) and using Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) to machine-code ethnographic accounts of morality from an additional 196 societies (the entire Human Relations Area Files, or HRAF, corpus). Again, we find evidence of most of the seven morals in most societies, across all cultural regions. The new method allows us to detect minor variations in morals across region and subsistence strategy. And we successfully validate the new machine-coding against the previous hand-coding. In light of these findings, MAC-D emerges as a theoretically-motivated, comprehensive, and validated tool for machine-reading moral corpora. We conclude by discussing the limitations of the current study, as well as prospects for future research.
Morality as Cooperation, Politics as Conflict
January 2024
|
Journal article
|
Social Psychological Bulletin
4408 Political Science, 44 Human Society
A broader theory of cooperation can better explain "purity".
October 2023
|
Journal article
|
The Behavioral and brain sciences
Self-control provides one cooperative explanation for "purity." Other types of cooperation provide additional explanations. For example, individuals compete for status by displaying high-value social and sexual traits, which are moralised because they reduce the mutual costs of conflict. As this theory predicts, sexually unattractive traits are perceived as morally bad, aside from self-control. Moral psychology will advance more quickly by drawing on all theories of cooperation.
Humans, Sexual Behavior, Cooperative Behavior, Morals
Gross values: Investigating the role of disgust in bioethics
February 2023
|
Journal article
|
Current Psychology
5205 Social and Personality Psychology, 52 Psychology, Behavioral and Social Science, Generic health relevance
Moral Messaging: Testing a Framing Technique during a Pandemic
January 2023
|
Journal article
|
Basic and Applied Social Psychology
52 Psychology, Infectious Diseases
Moral Molecules: Morality as a Combinatorial System
December 2022
|
Journal article
|
Review of Philosophy and Psychology
5003 Philosophy, 50 Philosophy and Religious Studies
Creative destruction in science
November 2020
|
Journal article
|
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
35 Commerce, Management, Tourism and Services, 3507 Strategy, Management and Organisational Behaviour, Generic health relevance
Morality is fundamentally an evolved solution to problems of social co‐operation
May 2020
|
Journal article
|
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute
Cooperative conservation: Seven ways to save the world
November 2019
|
Journal article
|
Conservation Science and Practice
Is it good to cooperate? Testing the theory of morality-as-cooperation in 60 societies
February 2019
|
Journal article
|
Current Anthropology
What is morality? And to what extent does it vary around the world? The theory of “morality-as-cooperation” argues that morality consists of a collection of biological and cultural solutions to the problems of cooperation recurrent in human social life. Morality-as-cooperation draws on the theory of non-zero-sum games to identify distinct problems of cooperation and their solutions, and it predicts that specific forms of cooperative behavior—including helping kin, helping your group, reciprocating, being brave, deferring to superiors, dividing disputed resources, and respecting prior possession—will be considered morally good wherever they arise, in all cultures. To test these predictions, we investigate the moral valence of these seven cooperative behaviors in the ethnographic records of 60 societies. We find that the moral valence of these behaviors is uniformly positive, and the majority of these cooperative morals are observed in the majority of cultures, with equal frequency across all regions of the world. We conclude that these seven cooperative behaviors are plausible candidates for universal moral rules, and that morality-as-cooperation could provide the unified theory of morality that anthropology has hitherto lacked.
Mapping Morality with a Compass: Testing the theory of ‘morality as cooper- ation’ with a new questionnaire
February 2019
|
Journal article
|
Journal of Research in Personality
A range of kindness activities boost happiness.
January 2019
|
Journal article
|
The Journal of social psychology
This experiment investigates the effects of a seven-day kindness activities intervention on changes in subjective happiness. The study was designed to test whether performing different types of kindness activities had differential effects on happiness. Our recent systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychological effects of kindness (Curry, et al. 2018) revealed that performing acts of kindness boosts happiness and well-being. However, we noted in that review that rarely had researchers specifically compared the effects of kindness to different recipients, such as to friends or to strangers. Thus in a single factorial design (n=683) we compare acts of kindness to strong social ties, weak social ties, novel acts of self kindness, and observing acts of kindness, against a no acts control group. The results indicate that performing kindness activities for seven days increases happiness. In addition, we report a positive correlation between the number of kind acts and increases in happiness. Neither effect differed across the experimental the groups, suggesting that kindness to strong ties, to weak ties, and to self, as well as observing acts of kindness, have equally positive effects on happiness.
Humans, Personal Satisfaction, Altruism, Happiness, Interpersonal Relations, Adult, Female, Male
Excavating the Foundations: Cognitive Adaptations for Multiple Moral Domains
December 2018
|
Journal article
|
Evolutionary Psychological Science
5202 Biological Psychology, 52 Psychology, Clinical Research
Understanding cooperation through fitness interdependence.
July 2018
|
Journal article
|
Nature human behaviour
Animals, Humans, Social Behavior, Cooperative Behavior, Marriage, Interpersonal Relations, Reproduction
How moments become movements: shared outrage, group cohesion, and the lion that went viral
May 2018
|
Journal article
|
Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution
Can moments of viral media activity transform into enduring activist movements? The killing of Cecil the lion by a trophy hunter in Zimbabwe in 2015 attracted global attention and generated enduring conservation activism in the form of monetary donations to the research unit that was studying him (WildCRU). Utilizing a longitudinal survey design, we found that intensely dysphoric reactions to Cecil's death triggered especially strong social cohesion (i.e., “identity fusion”) amongst donors. Over a 6-month period, identity fusion to WildCRU increased amongst donors. In addition, in line with an emerging psychological model of the experiential antecedents of identity fusion, cohesion amongst donors increased most for those who continued to reflect deeply on Cecil's death and felt his death to be a central event in their own lives. Our results highlight the profound capabilities of humans to commit resources to supporting others who are distant in space and time, unrelated culturally or biologically, and even (as in this case) belonging to another species altogether. In addition, our findings add to recent interdisciplinary work uncovering the precise social mechanisms by which intense group cohesion develops.
Happy to help? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of performing acts of kindness on the well-being of the actor
March 2018
|
Journal article
|
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
Do acts of kindness improve the well-being of the actor? Recent advances in the behavioural sciences have provided a number of explanations of human social, cooperative and altruistic behaviour. These theories predict that people will be ‘happy to help’ family, friends, community members, spouses, and even strangers under some conditions. Here we conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis of the experimental evidence that kindness interventions (for example, performing ‘random acts of kindness’) boost subjective well-being. Our initial search of the literature identified 489 articles; of which 24 (27 studies) met the inclusion criteria (total N = 4045). These 27 studies, some of which included multiple control conditions and dependent measures, yielded 52 effect sizes. Multi-level modeling revealed that the overall effect of kindness on the well-being of the actor is small-to-medium (δ = 0.28). The effect was not moderated by sex, age, type of participant, intervention, control condition or outcome measure. There was no indication of publication bias. We discuss the limitations of the current literature, and recommend that future research test more specific theories of kindness: taking kindness-specific individual differences into account; distinguishing between the effects of kindness to specific categories of people; and considering a wider range of proximal and distal outcomes. Such research will advance our understanding of the causes and consequences of kindness, and help practitioners to maximise the effectiveness of kindness interventions to improve well-being.
The conflict-resolution theory of virtue
October 2017
|
Chapter
|
Moral Psychology
There has been a long-standing debate in the history of moral thought over the nature of virtue—the enduring traits that are indicative of a good moral character. One tradition—represented by Aristotle, Cicero, Machiavelli, Nietzsche, and Hume—has celebrated the so-called “pagan” virtues of beauty, strength, courage, magnanimity, and leadership. Another tradition—represented particularly by theologians—has celebrated exactly the opposite set of traits: the so-called “Christian” virtues of humility, meekness, quietude, asceticism, and obedience (Berlin, 1997). But what are the virtues? Where do they come from? Why do they consist of these two apparently incompatible sets of traits? And why have they been considered moral?
SBTMR
Cross-cultural regularities in the cognitive architecture of pride
February 2017
|
Journal article
|
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Pride occurs in every known culture, appears early in development, is reliably triggered by achievements and formidability, and causes a characteristic display that is recognized everywhere. Here, we evaluate the theory that pride evolved to guide decisions relevant to pursuing actions that enhance valuation and respect for a person in the minds of others. By hypothesis, pride is a neurocomputational program tailored by selection to orchestrate cognition and behavior in the service of: (i) motivating the cost-effective pursuit of courses of action that would increase others' valuations and respect of the individual, (ii) motivating the advertisement of acts or characteristics whose recognition by others would lead them to enhance their evaluations of the individual, and (iii) mobilizing the individual to take advantage of the resulting enhanced social landscape. To modulate how much to invest in actions that might lead to enhanced evaluations by others, the pride system must forecast the magnitude of the evaluations the action would evoke in the audience and calibrate its activation proportionally. We tested this prediction in 16 countries across 4 continents (n = 2,085), for 25 acts and traits. As predicted, the pride intensity for a given act or trait closely tracks the valuations of audiences, local (mean r = +0.82) and foreign (mean r = +0.75). This relationship is specific to pride and does not generalize to other positive emotions that coactivate with pride but lack its audience-recalibrating function.
The complexity of jokes is limited by cognitive constraints on mentalizing
November 2015
|
Journal article
|
Human Nature
Although laughter is probably of deep evolutionary origin, the telling of jokes, being language-based, is likely to be of more recent origin within the human lineage. In language-based communication, speaker and listener are engaged in a process of mutually understanding each other’s intentions (mindstates), with a conversation minimally requiring three orders of intentionality. Mentalizing is cognitively more demanding than non-mentalizing cognition, and there is a well-attested limit at five orders in the levels of intentionality at which normal adult humans can work. Verbal jokes commonly involve commentary on the mindstates of third parties, and each such mindstate adds an additional level of intentionality and its corresponding cognitive load. We determined the number of mentalizing levels in a sample of jokes told by well-known professional comedians and show that most jokes involve either three or five orders of intentionality on the part of the comedian, depending on whether or not the joke involves other individuals’ mindstates. Within this limit there is a positive correlation between increasing levels of intentionality and subjective ratings of how funny the jokes are. The quality of jokes appears to peak when they include five or six levels of intentionality, which suggests that audiences appreciate higher mentalizing complexity whilst working within their natural cognitive constraints.
Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach
August 2015
|
Chapter
|
Evolution of Morality
<p>Your country is under attack and you are preparing to join the fight to defend it. Just then, your mother calls and tells you she is seriously ill and needs your help. Do you take care of your mother, or do you abandon her to fi ght for your country? You are a member of a sports team that always loses to a rival team. You have an opportunity to join that rival team. Do you take it? You borrow £10 from a wealthy friend. The friend forgets all about it. Do you give him the £10 back? You and another friend are walking along the street when you spot a £20 note on the ground. You bend down and pick it up. Do you offer to share it with your friend?</p> <br/> <p>In most people, these scenarios evoke a range of thoughts, feelings, emotions, and intuitions about what to do, what is the right thing to do, what one ought to do—what is the moralthing to do. What are these moral thoughts and feelings, where do they come from, how do they work, and what are they for? Scholars have struggled with these questions for millennia, and for many people the nature of morality is so baffling that they assume it must have a supernatural origin. </p> <br/> <p>The good news is that we now have a scientific answer to these questions. Previous approaches have noticed that morality has something to do with cooperation (see Table 1). But now it is possible to use the mathematical theory of cooperation—the theory of nonzero-sum games—to transform this commonplace into a precise and comprehensive theory, capable of making specific testable predictions about the nature of morality.</p> <br/> <p>In this chapter, I use game theory to identify the fundamental problems of human social life, and show how—in principle and in practice—they are solved. I argue hat it is the solutions to these problems that philosophers and others have called ‘morality’. Thus, morality turns out to be a collection of biological and cultural solutions to the problems of cooperation and conflict recurrent in human social life. I show how this theory of ‘morality as cooperation’ incorporates the best elements of previous theories, and moves beyond them to create a principled taxonomy of moral values of unprecedented depth and breadth. I derive from this theory testable predictions about the structure and content of moral thought and outline how they differ from those of rival theories. And I conclude that, because the debate between these theories can be resolved using standard scientifi c method, the study of morality has at last become a branch of science. </p>
SBTMR, psychology
Constituents of political cognition: Race, party politics, and the alliance detection system.
July 2015
|
Journal article
|
Cognition
Research suggests that the mind contains a set of adaptations for detecting alliances: an alliance detection system, which monitors for, encodes, and stores alliance information and then modifies the activation of stored alliance categories according to how likely they will predict behavior within a particular social interaction. Previous studies have established the activation of this system when exposed to explicit competition or cooperation between individuals. In the current studies we examine if shared political opinions produce these same effects. In particular, (1) if participants will spontaneously categorize individuals according to the parties they support, even when explicit cooperation and antagonism are absent, and (2) if party support is sufficiently powerful to decrease participants' categorization by an orthogonal but typically-diagnostic alliance cue (in this case the target's race). Evidence was found for both: Participants spontaneously and implicitly kept track of who supported which party, and when party cross-cut race-such that the race of targets was not predictive of party support-categorization by race was dramatically reduced. To verify that these results reflected the operation of a cognitive system for modifying the activation of alliance categories, and not just socially-relevant categories in general, an identical set of studies was also conducted with in which party was either crossed with sex or age (neither of which is predicted to be primarily an alliance category). As predicted, categorization by party occurred to the same degree, and there was no reduction in either categorization by sex or by age. All effects were replicated across two sets of between-subjects conditions. These studies provide the first direct empirical evidence that party politics engages the mind's systems for detecting alliances and establish two important social categorization phenomena: (1) that categorization by age is, like sex, not affected by alliance information and (2) that political contexts can reduce the degree to which individuals are represented in terms of their race.
Humans, Social Behavior, Social Identification, Cognition, Mental Recall, Neuropsychological Tests, Politics, Adolescent, Female, Male, Young Adult, Racial Groups
Social Welfare and the Psychology of Food Sharing: Short‐Term Hunger Increases Support for Social Welfare
December 2014
|
Journal article
|
Political Psychology
5205 Social and Personality Psychology, 52 Psychology, Behavioral and Social Science
Do Birds of a Feather Flock Together?: The Relationship between Similarity and Altruism in Social Networks
July 2013
|
Journal article
|
Human Nature
Cooperation requires that individuals are able to identify, and preferentially associate with, others who have compatible preferences and the shared background knowledge needed to solve interpersonal coordination problems. The present study investigates the nature of such similarity within social networks, asking: What do friends have in common? And what is the relationship between similarity and altruism? The results show that similarity declines with frequency of contact; similarity in general is a significant predictor of altruism and emotional closeness; and, specifically, sharing a sense of humor, hobbies and interests, moral beliefs, and being from the same area are the best predictors. These results shed light on the structure of relationships within networks and provide a possible checklist for predicting attitudes toward strangers, and in-group identification.
Male, SBTMR, Social Perception, Humans, Social Values, Friends, Social Identification, Great Britain, Emotions, Social Support, Interpersonal Relations, Female, Altruism, Adult, Cooperative Behavior, Linear Models, Morals
Sharing a joke: The effects of a similar sense of humor on affiliation and altruism
Altruism in social networks: Evidence for a 'kinship premium'.
May 2012
|
Journal article
|
British Journal of Psychology
Why and under what conditions are individuals altruistic to family and friends in their social networks? Evolutionary psychology suggests that such behaviour is primarily the product of adaptations for kin- and reciprocal altruism, dependent on the degree of genetic relatedness and exchange of benefits, respectively. For this reason, individuals are expected to be more altruistic to family members than to friends: whereas family members can be the recipients of kin and reciprocal altruism, friends can be the recipients of reciprocal altruism only. However, there is a question about how the effect of kinship is implemented at the proximate psychological level. One possibility is that kinship contributes to some general measure of relationship quality (such as 'emotional closeness'), which in turn explains altruism. Another possibility is that the effect of kinship is independent of relationship quality. The present study tests between these two possibilities. Participants (N= 111) completed a self-report questionnaire about their willingness to be altruistic, and their emotional closeness, to 12 family members and friends at different positions in their extended social networks. As expected, altruism was greater for family than friends, and greater for more central layers of the network. Crucially, the results showed that kinship made a significant unique contribution to altruism, even when controlling for the effects of emotional closeness. Thus, participants were more altruistic towards kin than would be expected if altruism was dependent on emotional closeness alone - a phenomenon we label a 'kinship premium'. These results have implications for the ongoing debate about the extent to which kin relations and friendships are distinct kinds of social relationships, and how to measure the 'strength of ties' in social networks.
Male, SBTMR, Humans, Friends, Social Networking, Young Adult, Self Report, Female, Family, Altruism, Adolescent
‘Putting Ourselves in the Other Fellow’ s Shoes’ : The Role of ‘Theory of Mind’ in Solving Coordination Problems
January 2012
|
Journal article
|
Journal of Cognition and Culture
5205 Social and Personality Psychology, 52 Psychology, Mental Health, 2.3 Psychological, social and economic factors, Mental health
Altruism in networks: the effect of connections.
October 2011
|
Journal article
|
Biology letters
Why are individuals altruistic to their friends? Theory suggests that individual, relationship and network factors will all influence the levels of altruism; but to date, the effects of social network structure have received relatively little attention. The present study uses a novel correlational design to test the prediction that an individual will be more altruistic to friends who are well-connected to the individual's other friends. The result shows that, as predicted, even when controlling for a range of individual and relationship factors, the network factor (number of connections) makes a significant contribution to altruism, thus showing that individuals are more likely to be altruistic to better-connected members of their social networks. The implications of incorporating network structure into studies of altruism are discussed.
Humans, Altruism, Interpersonal Relations, Adult, Middle Aged, Female, Male
The psychopath’s dilemma: The effects of psychopathic personality traits in one-shot games
April 2011
|
Journal article
|
Personality and Individual Differences
52 Psychology, 5205 Social and Personality Psychology, 5201 Applied and Developmental Psychology, Mental Health
Patience is a virtue: Cooperative people have lower discount rates
February 2008
|
Journal article
|
Personality and Individual Differences
5202 Biological Psychology, 52 Psychology
Darwinism's fantastic voyage
January 2008
|
Journal article
|
LANCET
The integrative framework for the behavioural sciences has already been discovered, and it is the adaptationist approach
February 2007
|
Journal article
|
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
32 Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, 5202 Biological Psychology, 5204 Cognitive and Computational Psychology, 3209 Neurosciences, 52 Psychology
One good deed
December 2006
|
Journal article
|
Nature
Who's Afraid of the Naturalistic Fallacy?
January 2006
|
Journal article
|
Evolutionary Psychology
52 Psychology, 31 Biological Sciences, 44 Human Society
A change of mind?
May 2005
|
Journal article
|
Nature
SBTMR
Get real: evolution as metaphor and mechanism
February 2003
|
Journal article
|
The British Journal of Politics and International Relations
4408 Political Science, 44 Human Society
Evolutionary psychology: "fashionable ideology" or "new foundation"?